Skip to main content

Spectrum: Autism Research News

Rinse and repeat

by  /  22 May 2012
THIS ARTICLE IS MORE THAN FIVE YEARS OLD

This article is more than five years old. Autism research — and science in general — is constantly evolving, so older articles may contain information or theories that have been reevaluated since their original publication date.

You’ve probably heard the axiom about autism’s infamous variability: If you’ve met one child with autism, you’ve met one child with autism — meaning that every child shows a unique combination of atypical behaviors. The same could be said for a mouse carrying an autism-linked genetic glitch.

Mice may all look alike but, like people, they carry different combinations of background genes that may interact with a given mutated gene. They also have variable exposures in utero and experiences after birth that may influence the expression of genes. Even if all of these variables were the same, no two mice, like no two people, will respond to a given situation in exactly the same way.

And yet, most studies are designed as if mice were all the same.  

Researchers typically use only one ‘cohort,’ a group of about three dozen mice, for a given set of experiments. In the handful of studies that have used more than one cohort — the latest of which came out 9 May in the Journal of Neuroscience — sometimes the results hold up, and sometimes they don’t.

“From my reading of the literature, the field has been seriously muddied by the current lack of required standards for at least one replication before publication — and before press releases,” says Crawley, chief of behavioral neuroscience at the National Institute of Mental Health, and lead investigator of the new report.

The report gives a perfect example of the many flavors of variability in mouse models. Crawley and her colleagues looked at mice carrying a mutation in one copy of SHANK3, a gene that’s strongly linked to autism.

The mice debuted in 2010 (with Crawley as one of the authors), and the new study validates some previous findings. For example, it confirms that these mice show normal social behaviors as adults and mild deficits during juvenile social interactions.

The new study also looked at a wide range of behaviors in three independent cohorts. The researchers found that, although object recognition and motor performance are consistent among the groups, repetitive self-grooming, abnormal ultrasonic vocalizations and water-maze learning are not.

It’s pretty clear, then, that mouse studies need to be repeated — in multiple cohorts and strains. Crawley’s lab routinely creates two cohorts of mice for each experiment. If the results don’t match, the researchers make a third.

These extra steps are no doubt more time-consuming and expensive, but without them, the data don’t mean much. If you study one mouse cohort of autism, you study one mouse cohort of autism.